full transcript

From the Ted Talk by David Puts: To find your perfect mate, think like an evolutionist

Unscramble the Blue Letters

Hi there. I could be wrong, but I think this talk may have the dctontsiiin of being the one talk in this series that ends with oagsrm. (Laughter) But let's not get ahead of ourselves. (leutahgr) Have you ever thought about the fact that you're here, alive on this penlat because every one of your ancestors rceuedorpd? Every one, in an uenobrkn chain, all the way back to the first life on this planet, over three and a half billion years ago. That's a lot of reproducing. And for the past billion yraes, your ancestors reproduced sexually. So sex is a pretty big deal. But you probably knew that. But let's talk about haumn mating. Why does human mating take the fomrs that it does? Why are we attracted to certain people? Why do we sometimes form long-term romantic relationships? Why do we sometimes cheat? Now I don't mean why consciously do we do these things. I don't mean what happens in the brian to cause it. I mean, why did we evolve these feelings and these behaviors? In other words, how did the uinerdylng brain sretrtuucs and brain chemistry contribute to our ancestors' reproductive success so that those traits got passed on into the present generation while others didn't. Answering evolutionary qeiosntus like this is like being a crime scene investigator, we're left with the evidence, and we have to try to establish what happened. So let's go back six or seven moliiln years ago to our early ancestors. This is right after the split between our lienage and the lineage that would eventually give rise to chimpanzees. Now these were small brained apes, they waelkd on two legs, and males probably fought each other for mating opportunities. We know this because males fight for mates in all of our closest living relatives, chimpanzees, orangutans, gorillas. And because males are larger than females when they fight for mates. And the fossil record indicates that our male atncrsoes were larger than females. So males tend to be larger, more mlsacuur, stronger, more physically aggressive, when they fight for mates. Our species has all the hallmarks of a species that's experienced an evolutionary history of male figtnhig for mates. For example, men have, on average, 60 prnceet more muscle mass, and 75 percent more ueppr body muscle mass, and those differences in musculature tlsnraate into large sex differences in strength. The average man is segrnotr than 99.9% of women. These are data on hand strength, which is a good predictor of overall upper body strength, on over 600 men and women. And as you can see, there's a large sex difference. And in fact, not one of almost 400 women had as strong of a hand strength as the average man. So, men can open jars. (Laughter) And move furniture or at least two things that we're good for. Who cares, right? The answer is that men care. Men, especially young men, seem really concerned about figuring out who's the toughest or strongest, or the most physically floabrmide, and sometimes they devise elaborate ways for determining this. From early development, boys and men are more physically agsseigvre than girls and women all over the wolrd, and this aggression sometimes rusetls in violence. Men have a virtual monopoly on same-sex homicides. In other wodrs, men are vastly more likely to kill each other than women are to kill each other. These are data from every society from every time period in history for which data were available when the authors compiled them, on proportion of same-sex homicides that are male killing male. And as you can see, the percentage is always close to 100%. On average, 95% of same-sex homicides are committed by males, and itapmlnotry, these don't include war killings, which would bring the percentages even closer to 100%. And from what evidence that we have, a dominance among men translates into mating and reproductive opportunities. So we're a species that's experienced an evolutionary history in which our male ancestors won mating opportunities through the use or threat of force. In that regard, our apple has not fallen far from the evolutionary tree. But in other ways, human mating and reproduction are profoundly different from what we see in our close relatives, and they've changed a lot since our early ancestors. For example, males in canpizhmees, orangutans and gorillas, spend time and effort competing for mates, but don't spend much time with individual females and don't provide resources. They don't provide food for their offspring. So that's a big change. Although most human sceiiteos allow polygamous marriage, that is one man married to more than one woman, even within polygamous societies, most marriages are monogamous. And in the average hunter-gatherer society, almost 80% of meairrd women are monogamous, so that's different. And, importantly, men pivorde resources for their mates and offspring. So how did we get there? Well, in species where males fghit each other for mates, dominant males indicated by the larger, dekrar male symbols here, tend to have more mating opportunities, and hence more offspring. And subordinate males tend to have fewer mating opneirtptoius and are more likely to fail to reproduce. So this sets up an interesting situation, because for subordinate males, it would be advantageous to aetpmtt monogamy rather than winning lots of mating opportunities. One mate is better than none. The poblrem is that in general, subordinate males cannot defend females from dominant males, and besides, females tend to perfer mating with dominant males for the genetic benefits, poiundcrg stronger, hieeltahr offspring. So what changed all of this was probably several tnorniiatss hnpniepag together around the same time. By about two and a half million years ago, we had started to ipractorone more meat into our diet. We know this from various lines of evidence, including - this is cool - stnoe tool cut makrs on animal bones dated to 2.5 million years ago. That's cool. I love this stuff! And then by about 2 million years ago, brain size really started to increase, and with that came a lgenhtnenig of the juvenile period, so now kids became both really coslty and costly for a long period of time. And this made male provisioning both possible and necessary. Possible because it's much eaesir to bring back calories, protein, fats, in the form of meat than trying to do that by transporting plant foods, and necessary because kids became so energetically costly that iiuiddnavl females would have had trouble providing resources for themselves and their offspring. And when we look at medron hunter-gatherer societies, that's what we see. These are aggregate data across several hunter-gatherer societies on net daily calories. Are you bringing in more calories than you consume, through foraging, or are you consuming more than you bring in? And the green bars are net daily caloric surplus, in other words, bringing in more than you consume. And the red bars are a deficit, so you're cnsouimng more than you bring in. And you'll notice that men from about 20 years of age to 60 years of age are operating at a daily caloric surplus. They bring in, generally through hunting, more calories than they can consume, and these calories are distributed. If it's lgrae game, it's gelrnealy distributed equally to everybody in the village or camp. Smaller iemts can be brought back to individual family, but this contrasts with what's going on with women, in their reproductive years, they're operating at a dliay ciroalc deficit. gitoetasn, lactation, carrying babies, are eletmrexy costly energetically and limit one's ability to forage efficiently. So male pnoiioirnsvg, both possible through hunting, and necessary. And this change had profound iatmpcs on human miantg and reproduction. In a sense, it tepipd the balance for fealmes. So now it was sometimes worth mating with a subordinate male, even if he may not possess the best genes, if he provided resources. And this is baboon pornography. (Laughter) I probably should have warned you there'd be monkey porn. This is from PlayBaboon Magazine. arglhit, I'm going to stop with the jokes. This is a female baboon in estrus, so her genitals are seowlln, and this happens in a lot of primate species. Females' appearance changes over the cycle, and becomes more attractive and this initces male competition for females during the fertile part of the cycle, with dnaimnot males tending to monopolize copulations, clsoer to ovulation. Well. We don't look like this. And you knew that. But what you might not know is that women's attractiveness does chagne over the cycle. My lab, and others, have swohn that women's faces, voices, even odors, are more attractive to men during the fertile part of the cycle. But these changes are extremely slbtue. And compared to other primates, the evidence indicates that we've evolved to suppress cues to ovtulaoin. That in a sense, ovulation is concealed in humans. But think about what impact this would have. This would mean that dominant males would not be able to monopolize coatnplouis near ovulation. It would protect the pair bond from invasion by a dominant male. So that a male in a pair would have more confidence that he was the father of the offspring. The couple is having sex throughout the cycle. And this is unique to human mating, we don't see it in many other primates. We have sex throughout the cycle. And so this would essentially increase a male's confidence in ptnertiay, because a dominant or some other male wouldn't be able to target the female and bully their way in at the fertile point in the cylce. And this would have ianmtrpot implications for parental investment, in particular, males providing resources for their offspring. Because across speecis, when maels provide resources for offspring, they target those resources toward their own biological offspring, and they avoid investing in the offspring of unrelated males. And so the evolution of male care for offspring and investing in rsecueors and offspring, pair bonding, and concealed ovulation, went very much hand-in-hand over our eotvuloin. We have also evolved a specialized psychology for foirmng long-term ritamnoc rhnitpiolseas with the pistiobsliy of investing in opfsnfrig together. We fall in love. All around the world, people prefer mates who are kind and generous and capable and willing to care for mates and offspring. In one of the largest cross-cultural sedtuis of human mate preferences ever conducted, covering 33 countries shown in red here, the single most important mate choice criterion to both men and wmoen, was mutual love and attraction. But as you also know, people are not always perfectly faithful to their mates. And in particular, women sometimes face a tradeoff between good genes and investment. Women sometimes find themselves in relationships with men who may be cnriag providers, but may not possess the best quality genes for offspring making them strong and healthy. And several features of women's mating psychology seem to have evolevd, in part, to resolve this trade off. And I mean, reictnruig genes, if you will, from outside of the long-term relationship. For example, women have more seaxul faanistes about men other than their long-term partner, during the fertile part of the cycle, and that's particularly true if the long-term partner has physical signs of being lower in genetic quality, like he's less physically artticavte. I think that's interesting. (Laughter) That's why I'm talking about it, I hope you do too. And women's mate pecefrneres similarly change over the cycle so that they prefer more dominant, more masculine males during the flritee part of the cycle. These are results of a study that I conducted on women's preferences for men's veicos. And I used ctepmuor sfwatroe to manipulate recordings of men's voices to make them sound either more masculine or more dominant, or more subordinate, more feminine. And I had women rate them on how attractive would this man be for a short-term, purely sexual relationship, and for a long-term committed relationship? And I also got information about where women were in their cycles. Were they in the fertile or non-fertile part of the cycle? These were all women not taking hormonal contraception. And what I found was that women preferred a more masculine, dominant-sounding voice, siflaplceciy in the fertile point of the cycle, and only for a sexual relationship versus a long-term cmttmeiod relationship. Now, this sounds like science fiction, but it's science, fact. Because this result has been shown lots of times across a variety of domains from women's preferences for men's voices, that this result was replicated by another lab. Women's preferences for men's faces, bodies, odors, and even behavior. Well, I said that we would get to orgasm. (Laughter) And we're there. I just want to strat by saying I'm for it. (Laughter) I'm pro-orgasm. I think more people should have more orgasms. But from a snfiitceic perspective, women's orgasm is especially fascinating because there's eevndcie indicating that it increases the probability that conception will result from an act of sex. There's evidence that it brings sperm up through the female reproductive tract and toward the egg. And think about what the ioitncplmias here could be. If women were more likely to have orgasms with some men than others, then this could be a mechanism by which they choose, not cuosonsicly, to be fertilized by some males and not others. And wouldn't you picdert that women would be more likely to have orgasms with males of high genetic quality? And in fact, a study by my lab published just a couple of years ago found that women reported more orgasms, earlier teimd orgasms, that is, they were easier to achieve, they aecehivd them more quickly, when they were having sex when their mate was more masculine and more dominant, and what's interesting is that this was true only for their orgasms from sexual intercourse, but not from other partnered sexual behaviors. I'll let you use your imagination what those might be. So we've seen that thinking like an eiolnuistovt can enable us to predict things about ourselves that we did not already know, and would not likely have guessed for a long time. We didn't know that women's mate preferences changed over the cycle. Until evolutionary thinking led us to that discovery. So that's one point that I want to make. But we've also seen how evolutionary thinking can clarify and unite dseivre parts of the human experience, and help us understand the best and the worst of ourselves, from violence and aggression and infidelity to men's care for their children, sexual attraction, sexual pleasure, and even the strength and fragility of romantic love. Thank you. (Applause)

Open Cloze

Hi there. I could be wrong, but I think this talk may have the ___________ of being the one talk in this series that ends with ______. (Laughter) But let's not get ahead of ourselves. (________) Have you ever thought about the fact that you're here, alive on this ______ because every one of your ancestors __________? Every one, in an ________ chain, all the way back to the first life on this planet, over three and a half billion years ago. That's a lot of reproducing. And for the past billion _____, your ancestors reproduced sexually. So sex is a pretty big deal. But you probably knew that. But let's talk about _____ mating. Why does human mating take the _____ that it does? Why are we attracted to certain people? Why do we sometimes form long-term romantic relationships? Why do we sometimes cheat? Now I don't mean why consciously do we do these things. I don't mean what happens in the _____ to cause it. I mean, why did we evolve these feelings and these behaviors? In other words, how did the __________ brain __________ and brain chemistry contribute to our ancestors' reproductive success so that those traits got passed on into the present generation while others didn't. Answering evolutionary _________ like this is like being a crime scene investigator, we're left with the evidence, and we have to try to establish what happened. So let's go back six or seven _______ years ago to our early ancestors. This is right after the split between our _______ and the lineage that would eventually give rise to chimpanzees. Now these were small brained apes, they ______ on two legs, and males probably fought each other for mating opportunities. We know this because males fight for mates in all of our closest living relatives, chimpanzees, orangutans, gorillas. And because males are larger than females when they fight for mates. And the fossil record indicates that our male _________ were larger than females. So males tend to be larger, more ________, stronger, more physically aggressive, when they fight for mates. Our species has all the hallmarks of a species that's experienced an evolutionary history of male ________ for mates. For example, men have, on average, 60 _______ more muscle mass, and 75 percent more _____ body muscle mass, and those differences in musculature _________ into large sex differences in strength. The average man is ________ than 99.9% of women. These are data on hand strength, which is a good predictor of overall upper body strength, on over 600 men and women. And as you can see, there's a large sex difference. And in fact, not one of almost 400 women had as strong of a hand strength as the average man. So, men can open jars. (Laughter) And move furniture or at least two things that we're good for. Who cares, right? The answer is that men care. Men, especially young men, seem really concerned about figuring out who's the toughest or strongest, or the most physically __________, and sometimes they devise elaborate ways for determining this. From early development, boys and men are more physically __________ than girls and women all over the _____, and this aggression sometimes _______ in violence. Men have a virtual monopoly on same-sex homicides. In other _____, men are vastly more likely to kill each other than women are to kill each other. These are data from every society from every time period in history for which data were available when the authors compiled them, on proportion of same-sex homicides that are male killing male. And as you can see, the percentage is always close to 100%. On average, 95% of same-sex homicides are committed by males, and ___________, these don't include war killings, which would bring the percentages even closer to 100%. And from what evidence that we have, a dominance among men translates into mating and reproductive opportunities. So we're a species that's experienced an evolutionary history in which our male ancestors won mating opportunities through the use or threat of force. In that regard, our apple has not fallen far from the evolutionary tree. But in other ways, human mating and reproduction are profoundly different from what we see in our close relatives, and they've changed a lot since our early ancestors. For example, males in ___________, orangutans and gorillas, spend time and effort competing for mates, but don't spend much time with individual females and don't provide resources. They don't provide food for their offspring. So that's a big change. Although most human _________ allow polygamous marriage, that is one man married to more than one woman, even within polygamous societies, most marriages are monogamous. And in the average hunter-gatherer society, almost 80% of _______ women are monogamous, so that's different. And, importantly, men _______ resources for their mates and offspring. So how did we get there? Well, in species where males _____ each other for mates, dominant males indicated by the larger, ______ male symbols here, tend to have more mating opportunities, and hence more offspring. And subordinate males tend to have fewer mating _____________ and are more likely to fail to reproduce. So this sets up an interesting situation, because for subordinate males, it would be advantageous to _______ monogamy rather than winning lots of mating opportunities. One mate is better than none. The _______ is that in general, subordinate males cannot defend females from dominant males, and besides, females tend to ______ mating with dominant males for the genetic benefits, _________ stronger, _________ offspring. So what changed all of this was probably several ___________ _________ together around the same time. By about two and a half million years ago, we had started to ___________ more meat into our diet. We know this from various lines of evidence, including - this is cool - _____ tool cut _____ on animal bones dated to 2.5 million years ago. That's cool. I love this stuff! And then by about 2 million years ago, brain size really started to increase, and with that came a ___________ of the juvenile period, so now kids became both really ______ and costly for a long period of time. And this made male provisioning both possible and necessary. Possible because it's much ______ to bring back calories, protein, fats, in the form of meat than trying to do that by transporting plant foods, and necessary because kids became so energetically costly that __________ females would have had trouble providing resources for themselves and their offspring. And when we look at ______ hunter-gatherer societies, that's what we see. These are aggregate data across several hunter-gatherer societies on net daily calories. Are you bringing in more calories than you consume, through foraging, or are you consuming more than you bring in? And the green bars are net daily caloric surplus, in other words, bringing in more than you consume. And the red bars are a deficit, so you're _________ more than you bring in. And you'll notice that men from about 20 years of age to 60 years of age are operating at a daily caloric surplus. They bring in, generally through hunting, more calories than they can consume, and these calories are distributed. If it's _____ game, it's _________ distributed equally to everybody in the village or camp. Smaller _____ can be brought back to individual family, but this contrasts with what's going on with women, in their reproductive years, they're operating at a _____ _______ deficit. _________, lactation, carrying babies, are _________ costly energetically and limit one's ability to forage efficiently. So male ____________, both possible through hunting, and necessary. And this change had profound _______ on human ______ and reproduction. In a sense, it ______ the balance for _______. So now it was sometimes worth mating with a subordinate male, even if he may not possess the best genes, if he provided resources. And this is baboon pornography. (Laughter) I probably should have warned you there'd be monkey porn. This is from PlayBaboon Magazine. _______, I'm going to stop with the jokes. This is a female baboon in estrus, so her genitals are _______, and this happens in a lot of primate species. Females' appearance changes over the cycle, and becomes more attractive and this _______ male competition for females during the fertile part of the cycle, with ________ males tending to monopolize copulations, ______ to ovulation. Well. We don't look like this. And you knew that. But what you might not know is that women's attractiveness does ______ over the cycle. My lab, and others, have _____ that women's faces, voices, even odors, are more attractive to men during the fertile part of the cycle. But these changes are extremely ______. And compared to other primates, the evidence indicates that we've evolved to suppress cues to _________. That in a sense, ovulation is concealed in humans. But think about what impact this would have. This would mean that dominant males would not be able to monopolize ___________ near ovulation. It would protect the pair bond from invasion by a dominant male. So that a male in a pair would have more confidence that he was the father of the offspring. The couple is having sex throughout the cycle. And this is unique to human mating, we don't see it in many other primates. We have sex throughout the cycle. And so this would essentially increase a male's confidence in _________, because a dominant or some other male wouldn't be able to target the female and bully their way in at the fertile point in the _____. And this would have _________ implications for parental investment, in particular, males providing resources for their offspring. Because across _______, when _____ provide resources for offspring, they target those resources toward their own biological offspring, and they avoid investing in the offspring of unrelated males. And so the evolution of male care for offspring and investing in _________ and offspring, pair bonding, and concealed ovulation, went very much hand-in-hand over our _________. We have also evolved a specialized psychology for _______ long-term ________ _____________ with the ___________ of investing in _________ together. We fall in love. All around the world, people prefer mates who are kind and generous and capable and willing to care for mates and offspring. In one of the largest cross-cultural _______ of human mate preferences ever conducted, covering 33 countries shown in red here, the single most important mate choice criterion to both men and _____, was mutual love and attraction. But as you also know, people are not always perfectly faithful to their mates. And in particular, women sometimes face a tradeoff between good genes and investment. Women sometimes find themselves in relationships with men who may be ______ providers, but may not possess the best quality genes for offspring making them strong and healthy. And several features of women's mating psychology seem to have _______, in part, to resolve this trade off. And I mean, __________ genes, if you will, from outside of the long-term relationship. For example, women have more ______ _________ about men other than their long-term partner, during the fertile part of the cycle, and that's particularly true if the long-term partner has physical signs of being lower in genetic quality, like he's less physically __________. I think that's interesting. (Laughter) That's why I'm talking about it, I hope you do too. And women's mate ___________ similarly change over the cycle so that they prefer more dominant, more masculine males during the _______ part of the cycle. These are results of a study that I conducted on women's preferences for men's ______. And I used ________ ________ to manipulate recordings of men's voices to make them sound either more masculine or more dominant, or more subordinate, more feminine. And I had women rate them on how attractive would this man be for a short-term, purely sexual relationship, and for a long-term committed relationship? And I also got information about where women were in their cycles. Were they in the fertile or non-fertile part of the cycle? These were all women not taking hormonal contraception. And what I found was that women preferred a more masculine, dominant-sounding voice, ____________ in the fertile point of the cycle, and only for a sexual relationship versus a long-term _________ relationship. Now, this sounds like science fiction, but it's science, fact. Because this result has been shown lots of times across a variety of domains from women's preferences for men's voices, that this result was replicated by another lab. Women's preferences for men's faces, bodies, odors, and even behavior. Well, I said that we would get to orgasm. (Laughter) And we're there. I just want to _____ by saying I'm for it. (Laughter) I'm pro-orgasm. I think more people should have more orgasms. But from a __________ perspective, women's orgasm is especially fascinating because there's ________ indicating that it increases the probability that conception will result from an act of sex. There's evidence that it brings sperm up through the female reproductive tract and toward the egg. And think about what the ____________ here could be. If women were more likely to have orgasms with some men than others, then this could be a mechanism by which they choose, not ___________, to be fertilized by some males and not others. And wouldn't you _______ that women would be more likely to have orgasms with males of high genetic quality? And in fact, a study by my lab published just a couple of years ago found that women reported more orgasms, earlier _____ orgasms, that is, they were easier to achieve, they ________ them more quickly, when they were having sex when their mate was more masculine and more dominant, and what's interesting is that this was true only for their orgasms from sexual intercourse, but not from other partnered sexual behaviors. I'll let you use your imagination what those might be. So we've seen that thinking like an ____________ can enable us to predict things about ourselves that we did not already know, and would not likely have guessed for a long time. We didn't know that women's mate preferences changed over the cycle. Until evolutionary thinking led us to that discovery. So that's one point that I want to make. But we've also seen how evolutionary thinking can clarify and unite _______ parts of the human experience, and help us understand the best and the worst of ourselves, from violence and aggression and infidelity to men's care for their children, sexual attraction, sexual pleasure, and even the strength and fragility of romantic love. Thank you. (Applause)

Solution

  1. attempt
  2. laughter
  3. consciously
  4. formidable
  5. brain
  6. forms
  7. human
  8. lineage
  9. modern
  10. timed
  11. start
  12. healthier
  13. ovulation
  14. paternity
  15. darker
  16. women
  17. evolution
  18. societies
  19. world
  20. shown
  21. marks
  22. males
  23. fertile
  24. species
  25. achieved
  26. results
  27. impacts
  28. individual
  29. cycle
  30. upper
  31. producing
  32. items
  33. planet
  34. forming
  35. diverse
  36. predict
  37. ancestors
  38. evolutionist
  39. chimpanzees
  40. daily
  41. implications
  42. reproduced
  43. fantasies
  44. large
  45. transitions
  46. alright
  47. stone
  48. voices
  49. extremely
  50. lengthening
  51. provisioning
  52. scientific
  53. married
  54. muscular
  55. structures
  56. costly
  57. specifically
  58. stronger
  59. percent
  60. words
  61. questions
  62. attractive
  63. recruiting
  64. females
  65. studies
  66. tipped
  67. swollen
  68. romantic
  69. million
  70. relationships
  71. prefer
  72. problem
  73. caloric
  74. underlying
  75. opportunities
  76. dominant
  77. fighting
  78. offspring
  79. provide
  80. committed
  81. aggressive
  82. orgasm
  83. importantly
  84. important
  85. years
  86. consuming
  87. incorporate
  88. fight
  89. unbroken
  90. software
  91. closer
  92. happening
  93. generally
  94. subtle
  95. incites
  96. walked
  97. possibility
  98. computer
  99. easier
  100. resources
  101. change
  102. evolved
  103. distinction
  104. evidence
  105. translate
  106. mating
  107. caring
  108. gestation
  109. copulations
  110. sexual
  111. preferences

Original Text

Hi there. I could be wrong, but I think this talk may have the distinction of being the one talk in this series that ends with orgasm. (Laughter) But let's not get ahead of ourselves. (Laughter) Have you ever thought about the fact that you're here, alive on this planet because every one of your ancestors reproduced? Every one, in an unbroken chain, all the way back to the first life on this planet, over three and a half billion years ago. That's a lot of reproducing. And for the past billion years, your ancestors reproduced sexually. So sex is a pretty big deal. But you probably knew that. But let's talk about human mating. Why does human mating take the forms that it does? Why are we attracted to certain people? Why do we sometimes form long-term romantic relationships? Why do we sometimes cheat? Now I don't mean why consciously do we do these things. I don't mean what happens in the brain to cause it. I mean, why did we evolve these feelings and these behaviors? In other words, how did the underlying brain structures and brain chemistry contribute to our ancestors' reproductive success so that those traits got passed on into the present generation while others didn't. Answering evolutionary questions like this is like being a crime scene investigator, we're left with the evidence, and we have to try to establish what happened. So let's go back six or seven million years ago to our early ancestors. This is right after the split between our lineage and the lineage that would eventually give rise to chimpanzees. Now these were small brained apes, they walked on two legs, and males probably fought each other for mating opportunities. We know this because males fight for mates in all of our closest living relatives, chimpanzees, orangutans, gorillas. And because males are larger than females when they fight for mates. And the fossil record indicates that our male ancestors were larger than females. So males tend to be larger, more muscular, stronger, more physically aggressive, when they fight for mates. Our species has all the hallmarks of a species that's experienced an evolutionary history of male fighting for mates. For example, men have, on average, 60 percent more muscle mass, and 75 percent more upper body muscle mass, and those differences in musculature translate into large sex differences in strength. The average man is stronger than 99.9% of women. These are data on hand strength, which is a good predictor of overall upper body strength, on over 600 men and women. And as you can see, there's a large sex difference. And in fact, not one of almost 400 women had as strong of a hand strength as the average man. So, men can open jars. (Laughter) And move furniture or at least two things that we're good for. Who cares, right? The answer is that men care. Men, especially young men, seem really concerned about figuring out who's the toughest or strongest, or the most physically formidable, and sometimes they devise elaborate ways for determining this. From early development, boys and men are more physically aggressive than girls and women all over the world, and this aggression sometimes results in violence. Men have a virtual monopoly on same-sex homicides. In other words, men are vastly more likely to kill each other than women are to kill each other. These are data from every society from every time period in history for which data were available when the authors compiled them, on proportion of same-sex homicides that are male killing male. And as you can see, the percentage is always close to 100%. On average, 95% of same-sex homicides are committed by males, and importantly, these don't include war killings, which would bring the percentages even closer to 100%. And from what evidence that we have, a dominance among men translates into mating and reproductive opportunities. So we're a species that's experienced an evolutionary history in which our male ancestors won mating opportunities through the use or threat of force. In that regard, our apple has not fallen far from the evolutionary tree. But in other ways, human mating and reproduction are profoundly different from what we see in our close relatives, and they've changed a lot since our early ancestors. For example, males in chimpanzees, orangutans and gorillas, spend time and effort competing for mates, but don't spend much time with individual females and don't provide resources. They don't provide food for their offspring. So that's a big change. Although most human societies allow polygamous marriage, that is one man married to more than one woman, even within polygamous societies, most marriages are monogamous. And in the average hunter-gatherer society, almost 80% of married women are monogamous, so that's different. And, importantly, men provide resources for their mates and offspring. So how did we get there? Well, in species where males fight each other for mates, dominant males indicated by the larger, darker male symbols here, tend to have more mating opportunities, and hence more offspring. And subordinate males tend to have fewer mating opportunities and are more likely to fail to reproduce. So this sets up an interesting situation, because for subordinate males, it would be advantageous to attempt monogamy rather than winning lots of mating opportunities. One mate is better than none. The problem is that in general, subordinate males cannot defend females from dominant males, and besides, females tend to prefer mating with dominant males for the genetic benefits, producing stronger, healthier offspring. So what changed all of this was probably several transitions happening together around the same time. By about two and a half million years ago, we had started to incorporate more meat into our diet. We know this from various lines of evidence, including - this is cool - stone tool cut marks on animal bones dated to 2.5 million years ago. That's cool. I love this stuff! And then by about 2 million years ago, brain size really started to increase, and with that came a lengthening of the juvenile period, so now kids became both really costly and costly for a long period of time. And this made male provisioning both possible and necessary. Possible because it's much easier to bring back calories, protein, fats, in the form of meat than trying to do that by transporting plant foods, and necessary because kids became so energetically costly that individual females would have had trouble providing resources for themselves and their offspring. And when we look at modern hunter-gatherer societies, that's what we see. These are aggregate data across several hunter-gatherer societies on net daily calories. Are you bringing in more calories than you consume, through foraging, or are you consuming more than you bring in? And the green bars are net daily caloric surplus, in other words, bringing in more than you consume. And the red bars are a deficit, so you're consuming more than you bring in. And you'll notice that men from about 20 years of age to 60 years of age are operating at a daily caloric surplus. They bring in, generally through hunting, more calories than they can consume, and these calories are distributed. If it's large game, it's generally distributed equally to everybody in the village or camp. Smaller items can be brought back to individual family, but this contrasts with what's going on with women, in their reproductive years, they're operating at a daily caloric deficit. Gestation, lactation, carrying babies, are extremely costly energetically and limit one's ability to forage efficiently. So male provisioning, both possible through hunting, and necessary. And this change had profound impacts on human mating and reproduction. In a sense, it tipped the balance for females. So now it was sometimes worth mating with a subordinate male, even if he may not possess the best genes, if he provided resources. And this is baboon pornography. (Laughter) I probably should have warned you there'd be monkey porn. This is from PlayBaboon Magazine. Alright, I'm going to stop with the jokes. This is a female baboon in estrus, so her genitals are swollen, and this happens in a lot of primate species. Females' appearance changes over the cycle, and becomes more attractive and this incites male competition for females during the fertile part of the cycle, with dominant males tending to monopolize copulations, closer to ovulation. Well. We don't look like this. And you knew that. But what you might not know is that women's attractiveness does change over the cycle. My lab, and others, have shown that women's faces, voices, even odors, are more attractive to men during the fertile part of the cycle. But these changes are extremely subtle. And compared to other primates, the evidence indicates that we've evolved to suppress cues to ovulation. That in a sense, ovulation is concealed in humans. But think about what impact this would have. This would mean that dominant males would not be able to monopolize copulations near ovulation. It would protect the pair bond from invasion by a dominant male. So that a male in a pair would have more confidence that he was the father of the offspring. The couple is having sex throughout the cycle. And this is unique to human mating, we don't see it in many other primates. We have sex throughout the cycle. And so this would essentially increase a male's confidence in paternity, because a dominant or some other male wouldn't be able to target the female and bully their way in at the fertile point in the cycle. And this would have important implications for parental investment, in particular, males providing resources for their offspring. Because across species, when males provide resources for offspring, they target those resources toward their own biological offspring, and they avoid investing in the offspring of unrelated males. And so the evolution of male care for offspring and investing in resources and offspring, pair bonding, and concealed ovulation, went very much hand-in-hand over our evolution. We have also evolved a specialized psychology for forming long-term romantic relationships with the possibility of investing in offspring together. We fall in love. All around the world, people prefer mates who are kind and generous and capable and willing to care for mates and offspring. In one of the largest cross-cultural studies of human mate preferences ever conducted, covering 33 countries shown in red here, the single most important mate choice criterion to both men and women, was mutual love and attraction. But as you also know, people are not always perfectly faithful to their mates. And in particular, women sometimes face a tradeoff between good genes and investment. Women sometimes find themselves in relationships with men who may be caring providers, but may not possess the best quality genes for offspring making them strong and healthy. And several features of women's mating psychology seem to have evolved, in part, to resolve this trade off. And I mean, recruiting genes, if you will, from outside of the long-term relationship. For example, women have more sexual fantasies about men other than their long-term partner, during the fertile part of the cycle, and that's particularly true if the long-term partner has physical signs of being lower in genetic quality, like he's less physically attractive. I think that's interesting. (Laughter) That's why I'm talking about it, I hope you do too. And women's mate preferences similarly change over the cycle so that they prefer more dominant, more masculine males during the fertile part of the cycle. These are results of a study that I conducted on women's preferences for men's voices. And I used computer software to manipulate recordings of men's voices to make them sound either more masculine or more dominant, or more subordinate, more feminine. And I had women rate them on how attractive would this man be for a short-term, purely sexual relationship, and for a long-term committed relationship? And I also got information about where women were in their cycles. Were they in the fertile or non-fertile part of the cycle? These were all women not taking hormonal contraception. And what I found was that women preferred a more masculine, dominant-sounding voice, specifically in the fertile point of the cycle, and only for a sexual relationship versus a long-term committed relationship. Now, this sounds like science fiction, but it's science, fact. Because this result has been shown lots of times across a variety of domains from women's preferences for men's voices, that this result was replicated by another lab. Women's preferences for men's faces, bodies, odors, and even behavior. Well, I said that we would get to orgasm. (Laughter) And we're there. I just want to start by saying I'm for it. (Laughter) I'm pro-orgasm. I think more people should have more orgasms. But from a scientific perspective, women's orgasm is especially fascinating because there's evidence indicating that it increases the probability that conception will result from an act of sex. There's evidence that it brings sperm up through the female reproductive tract and toward the egg. And think about what the implications here could be. If women were more likely to have orgasms with some men than others, then this could be a mechanism by which they choose, not consciously, to be fertilized by some males and not others. And wouldn't you predict that women would be more likely to have orgasms with males of high genetic quality? And in fact, a study by my lab published just a couple of years ago found that women reported more orgasms, earlier timed orgasms, that is, they were easier to achieve, they achieved them more quickly, when they were having sex when their mate was more masculine and more dominant, and what's interesting is that this was true only for their orgasms from sexual intercourse, but not from other partnered sexual behaviors. I'll let you use your imagination what those might be. So we've seen that thinking like an evolutionist can enable us to predict things about ourselves that we did not already know, and would not likely have guessed for a long time. We didn't know that women's mate preferences changed over the cycle. Until evolutionary thinking led us to that discovery. So that's one point that I want to make. But we've also seen how evolutionary thinking can clarify and unite diverse parts of the human experience, and help us understand the best and the worst of ourselves, from violence and aggression and infidelity to men's care for their children, sexual attraction, sexual pleasure, and even the strength and fragility of romantic love. Thank you. (Applause)

Frequently Occurring Word Combinations

ngrams of length 2

collocation frequency
human mating 4
million years 4
mating opportunities 4
dominant males 4
fertile part 4
provide resources 3
daily caloric 3
mate preferences 3
early ancestors 2
males fight 2
male ancestors 2
males tend 2
evolutionary history 2
upper body 2
large sex 2
average man 2
individual females 2
subordinate males 2
providing resources 2
net daily 2
fertile point 2
evolutionary thinking 2

Important Words

  1. ability
  2. achieve
  3. achieved
  4. act
  5. advantageous
  6. age
  7. aggregate
  8. aggression
  9. aggressive
  10. alive
  11. alright
  12. ancestors
  13. animal
  14. answer
  15. answering
  16. apes
  17. appearance
  18. applause
  19. apple
  20. attempt
  21. attracted
  22. attraction
  23. attractive
  24. attractiveness
  25. authors
  26. average
  27. avoid
  28. babies
  29. baboon
  30. balance
  31. bars
  32. behavior
  33. behaviors
  34. benefits
  35. big
  36. billion
  37. biological
  38. bodies
  39. body
  40. bond
  41. bonding
  42. bones
  43. boys
  44. brain
  45. brained
  46. bring
  47. bringing
  48. brings
  49. brought
  50. bully
  51. caloric
  52. calories
  53. camp
  54. capable
  55. care
  56. cares
  57. caring
  58. carrying
  59. chain
  60. change
  61. changed
  62. cheat
  63. chemistry
  64. children
  65. chimpanzees
  66. choice
  67. choose
  68. clarify
  69. close
  70. closer
  71. closest
  72. committed
  73. compared
  74. competing
  75. competition
  76. compiled
  77. computer
  78. concealed
  79. conception
  80. concerned
  81. conducted
  82. confidence
  83. consciously
  84. consume
  85. consuming
  86. contraception
  87. contrasts
  88. contribute
  89. cool
  90. copulations
  91. costly
  92. countries
  93. couple
  94. covering
  95. crime
  96. criterion
  97. cues
  98. cut
  99. cycle
  100. cycles
  101. daily
  102. darker
  103. data
  104. dated
  105. deal
  106. defend
  107. deficit
  108. determining
  109. development
  110. devise
  111. diet
  112. difference
  113. differences
  114. discovery
  115. distinction
  116. distributed
  117. diverse
  118. domains
  119. dominance
  120. dominant
  121. earlier
  122. early
  123. easier
  124. efficiently
  125. effort
  126. egg
  127. elaborate
  128. enable
  129. ends
  130. energetically
  131. equally
  132. essentially
  133. establish
  134. estrus
  135. eventually
  136. evidence
  137. evolution
  138. evolutionary
  139. evolutionist
  140. evolve
  141. evolved
  142. experience
  143. experienced
  144. extremely
  145. face
  146. faces
  147. fact
  148. fail
  149. faithful
  150. fall
  151. fallen
  152. family
  153. fantasies
  154. fascinating
  155. father
  156. fats
  157. features
  158. feelings
  159. female
  160. females
  161. feminine
  162. fertile
  163. fertilized
  164. fiction
  165. fight
  166. fighting
  167. figuring
  168. find
  169. food
  170. foods
  171. forage
  172. foraging
  173. force
  174. form
  175. formidable
  176. forming
  177. forms
  178. fossil
  179. fought
  180. fragility
  181. furniture
  182. game
  183. general
  184. generally
  185. generation
  186. generous
  187. genes
  188. genetic
  189. genitals
  190. gestation
  191. girls
  192. give
  193. good
  194. gorillas
  195. green
  196. guessed
  197. hallmarks
  198. hand
  199. happened
  200. happening
  201. healthier
  202. healthy
  203. high
  204. history
  205. homicides
  206. hope
  207. hormonal
  208. human
  209. humans
  210. hunting
  211. imagination
  212. impact
  213. impacts
  214. implications
  215. important
  216. importantly
  217. incites
  218. include
  219. including
  220. incorporate
  221. increase
  222. increases
  223. indicating
  224. individual
  225. infidelity
  226. information
  227. intercourse
  228. interesting
  229. invasion
  230. investigator
  231. investing
  232. investment
  233. items
  234. jars
  235. jokes
  236. juvenile
  237. kids
  238. kill
  239. killing
  240. killings
  241. kind
  242. knew
  243. lab
  244. lactation
  245. large
  246. larger
  247. largest
  248. laughter
  249. led
  250. left
  251. legs
  252. lengthening
  253. life
  254. limit
  255. lineage
  256. lines
  257. living
  258. long
  259. lot
  260. lots
  261. love
  262. magazine
  263. making
  264. male
  265. males
  266. man
  267. manipulate
  268. marks
  269. marriage
  270. marriages
  271. married
  272. masculine
  273. mass
  274. mate
  275. mates
  276. mating
  277. meat
  278. mechanism
  279. men
  280. million
  281. modern
  282. monkey
  283. monogamous
  284. monogamy
  285. monopolize
  286. monopoly
  287. move
  288. muscle
  289. muscular
  290. musculature
  291. mutual
  292. net
  293. notice
  294. odors
  295. offspring
  296. open
  297. operating
  298. opportunities
  299. orangutans
  300. orgasm
  301. orgasms
  302. ovulation
  303. pair
  304. parental
  305. part
  306. partner
  307. partnered
  308. parts
  309. passed
  310. paternity
  311. people
  312. percent
  313. percentage
  314. percentages
  315. perfectly
  316. period
  317. perspective
  318. physical
  319. physically
  320. planet
  321. plant
  322. playbaboon
  323. pleasure
  324. point
  325. polygamous
  326. porn
  327. pornography
  328. possess
  329. possibility
  330. predict
  331. predictor
  332. prefer
  333. preferences
  334. preferred
  335. present
  336. pretty
  337. primate
  338. primates
  339. probability
  340. problem
  341. producing
  342. profound
  343. profoundly
  344. proportion
  345. protect
  346. protein
  347. provide
  348. providers
  349. providing
  350. provisioning
  351. psychology
  352. published
  353. purely
  354. quality
  355. questions
  356. quickly
  357. rate
  358. record
  359. recordings
  360. recruiting
  361. red
  362. regard
  363. relationship
  364. relationships
  365. relatives
  366. replicated
  367. reported
  368. reproduce
  369. reproduced
  370. reproducing
  371. reproduction
  372. reproductive
  373. resolve
  374. resources
  375. result
  376. results
  377. rise
  378. romantic
  379. scene
  380. science
  381. scientific
  382. sense
  383. series
  384. sets
  385. sex
  386. sexual
  387. sexually
  388. shown
  389. signs
  390. similarly
  391. single
  392. situation
  393. size
  394. small
  395. smaller
  396. societies
  397. society
  398. software
  399. sound
  400. sounds
  401. specialized
  402. species
  403. specifically
  404. spend
  405. sperm
  406. split
  407. start
  408. started
  409. stone
  410. stop
  411. strength
  412. strong
  413. stronger
  414. strongest
  415. structures
  416. studies
  417. study
  418. subordinate
  419. subtle
  420. success
  421. suppress
  422. surplus
  423. swollen
  424. symbols
  425. talk
  426. talking
  427. target
  428. tend
  429. tending
  430. thinking
  431. thought
  432. threat
  433. time
  434. timed
  435. times
  436. tipped
  437. tool
  438. toughest
  439. tract
  440. trade
  441. tradeoff
  442. traits
  443. transitions
  444. translate
  445. translates
  446. transporting
  447. tree
  448. trouble
  449. true
  450. unbroken
  451. underlying
  452. understand
  453. unique
  454. unite
  455. unrelated
  456. upper
  457. variety
  458. vastly
  459. village
  460. violence
  461. virtual
  462. voice
  463. voices
  464. walked
  465. war
  466. warned
  467. ways
  468. winning
  469. woman
  470. women
  471. won
  472. words
  473. world
  474. worst
  475. worth
  476. wrong
  477. years
  478. young